Your data on MRCVSonline
The nature of the services provided by Vision Media means that we might obtain certain information about you.
Please read our Data Protection and Privacy Policy for details.

In addition, (with your consent) some parts of our website may store a 'cookie' in your browser for the purposes of
functionality or performance monitoring.
Click here to manage your settings.
If you would like to forward this story on to a friend, simply fill in the form below and click send.

Your friend's email:
Your email:
Your name:
 
 
Send Cancel

Feedback affects vocalisation in infant marmosets
Marmosets
The infant marmoset twins who received consistent and regular feedback, developed mature-sounding vocals faster than the twins who received less feedback.

Similarities in human and marmoset infant speech development

Researchers at Princeton University have drawn parallels between the effects of feedback on the development of human infant speech and that of infant marmoset monkeys.

The report, published in Current Biology, found that as the vocal development of infant marmosets sped up, the more frequently they received vocal feedback from adults.

Although marmoset calls do not match the complexity of human language, infants in both species start their vocal development by making relatively random sounds.

Asif Ghanzanfar, senior co-author of the study and professor of psychology at the Princeton Neuroscience Institute, said: “We wanted to find out whether the idea that monkeys don't do any learning during their vocal development is actually true. So we picked a species that we know really relies on vocalisations as its primary social signals. What we found in marmoset vocal development very closely parallels pre-linguistic vocal development in humans.

“When an infant blurts out something and the parent responds, that's a contingent response. And the more often a parent provides that contingent response, the faster the human infant will develop its vocalisations.”


To ascertain these findings, the research team arranged an experiment in which pairs of fraternal twin marmosets were observed, starting one day after their birth. For 40 minutes each day, the infant marmosets were separated from the adults and, for the first 10 minutes, their noises were recorded. For the remaining 30 minutes, researchers provided contingent feedback to the infant marmosets in the form of audio playback of their parent’s calls.

In each set of twins, one marmoset received regular feedback, to mirror the response of an attentive parent. At the same time, the other twin received less feedback in response to their calls. This experiment was repeated each day for the first two months of the infants lives, which equates to around two years, in marmoset years.

Despite the short period of time each day, the infant marmoset twins who received consistent and regular feedback, developed mature-sounding vocals faster than the twins who received less feedback.

Ghanzanfar stated: “This system of vocal learning production may be linked to the idea that an infant that more quickly produces adult-sounding calls is more likely to get care from a caregiver in a cooperative breeding environment where multiple individuals could be that caregiver, in addition to the parents. So it's not only this process of learning that's similar to humans; the whole reproductive strategy is similar to humans."

Ghanzanfar went on to say: "Vocal production learning isn't just about imitation. And you can no longer say that non-human primates show no evidence of vocal learning."

The research team will continue to collect data on marmosets, with their next goal to gather information on their neural activity when they are conversing with one another and calling to neighbours. Scientists believe that, in understanding marmoset communication, they may be able to further understand the evolution and development of speech.

Become a member or log in to add this story to your CPD history

Practices urged to audit neutering data

News Story 1
 RCVS Knowledge has called on vet practices to audit their post-operative neutering outcomes.

It follows the release of the 2024 NASAN benchmarking report, which collates data from neutering procedures performed on dogs, cats and rabbits.

The benchmarking report enables practices in the UK and Ireland to compare their post-operative outcomes to the national average. This includes the rate of patients lost to follow-up, which in 2024 increased to 23 per cent.

Anyone from the practice can submit the data using a free template. The deadline for next report is February 2026.

Visit the RCVS Knowledge website to complete an audit. 

Click here for more...
News Shorts
UK's BSE risk status downgraded

The WOAH has downgraded the UK's international risk status for BSE to 'negligible'.

Defra says that the UK's improved risk status recognises the reputation for having the highest standards for biosecurity. It adds that it demonstrates decades of rigorous animal control.

Outbreaks of Bovine Spongiform Encephalopathy, also known as mad cow disease, have previously resulted in bans on Britain's beef exports.

The UK's new status could lead to expanded trade and better confidence in British beef.

Christine Middlemiss, the UK's chief veterinary officer, said: "WOAH's recognition of the UK as negligible risk for BSE is a significant milestone and is a testament to the UK's strong biosecurity measures and the hard work and vigilance of farmers and livestock keepers across the country who have all played their part in managing the spread of this disease.